Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 2.450
Filtrar
1.
Nat Food ; 5(4): 281-287, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38605131

RESUMO

Food texture, along with taste and odour, is an important factor in determining food flavour. However, the physiological properties of oral texture perception require greater examination and definition. Here we explore recent trends and perspectives related to mouthfeel and its relevance in food flavour perception, with an emphasis on the biophysical point of view and methods. We propose that atomic force microscopy, combined with other biophysical techniques and more traditional food science approaches, offers a unique opportunity to study the mechanisms of mouthfeel at cellular and molecular levels. With this knowledge, food composition could be modified to develop healthier products by limiting salt, sugar, fat and calories while maintaining sensory qualities and consumer acceptance.


Assuntos
Microscopia de Força Atômica , Boca , Percepção Gustatória , Paladar , Microscopia de Força Atômica/métodos , Humanos , Paladar/fisiologia , Percepção Gustatória/fisiologia
2.
Aging Clin Exp Res ; 36(1): 84, 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38558357

RESUMO

This review critically assessed the existence of presbygeusia, i.e., the impairment in taste perception occurring in the elderly, as a natural part of the aging process and its potential clinical implications. Several factors might contribute to age-related taste alterations (TAs), including structural changes in taste buds, alterations in saliva composition, central nervous system changes, and oral microbiota dysbiosis. A comprehensive literature review was conducted to disentangle the effects of age from those of the several age-related diseases or conditions promoting TAs. Most of the included studies reported TAs in healthy elderly people, suggesting that presbygeusia is a relatively frequent condition associated with age-related changes in the absence of pathological conditions. However, the impact of TAs on dietary preferences and food choices among the elderly seems to be less relevant when compared to other factors, such as cultural, psychological, and social influences. In conclusion, presbygeusia exists even in the absence of comorbidities or drug side effects, but its impact on dietary choices in the elderly is likely modest.


Assuntos
Papilas Gustativas , Paladar , Humanos , Idoso , Paladar/fisiologia , Percepção Gustatória/fisiologia , Saliva/química , Saliva/fisiologia , Papilas Gustativas/fisiologia , Preferências Alimentares
3.
Curr Biol ; 34(7): R271-R272, 2024 Apr 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38593768

RESUMO

Taste is a sense that detects information about nutrients and toxins in foods. Of the five basic taste qualities, bitterness is associated with the detection of potentially harmful substances like plant alkaloids. In bony vertebrates, type 2 taste receptors (T2Rs), which are G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs), act as bitter taste receptors1,2. In vertebrates, six GPCR gene families are described as chemosensory receptor genes, encoding taste receptor families (T1Rs and T2Rs) and olfactory receptor families (ORs, V1Rs, V2Rs, and TAARs). These families of receptors have been found in all major jawed vertebrate lineages, except for the T2Rs, which are confined to bony vertebrates3. Therefore, T2Rs are believed to have emerged later than the other chemosensory receptor genes in the bony vertebrate lineage. So far, only the genomes of two cartilaginous fish species have been mined for TAS2R genes, which encode T2Rs4. Here, we identified novel T2Rs in elasmobranchs, namely selachimorphs (sharks) and batoids (rays, skates, and their close relatives) by an exhaustive search covering diverse cartilaginous fishes. Using functional and mRNA expression analyses, we demonstrate that their T2Rs are expressed in the oral taste buds and contribute to the detection of bitter compounds. This finding indicates the early origin of T2Rs in the common ancestor of jawed vertebrates.


Assuntos
Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G , Paladar , Animais , Paladar/fisiologia , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/genética , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/metabolismo , Vertebrados/genética , Vertebrados/metabolismo , Evolução Biológica , Peixes/genética , Percepção Gustatória
4.
Clin Oral Investig ; 28(4): 237, 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38558265

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study was to evaluate the effect of orthognathic surgery on taste sensation. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Thirty-five patients scheduled to undergo Le Fort I osteotomy (LFIO), sagittal split ramus osteotomy (SSRO), and bimaxillary surgery (BMS) were evaluated by administering localized and whole-mouth taste tests preoperatively and postoperatively at months 1, 3, and 6. The patients were asked to identify the quality of four basic tastes applied to six locations on the palate and tongue and to rate the taste intensities they perceived. Taste recognition thresholds and taste intesity scores were evaluted according to operation groups and follow-ups. RESULTS: There were significant decreases in the quinine HCl recognition thresholds at the postoperative follow-ups compared to the preoperative in LFIO patients (p = 0.043). There were significant decreases in sucrose taste intensity scores in the right posterolateral part of the tongue at months 3 and 6 compared to preoperative in SSRO patients (p = 0.046), and significant increases in quinine HCL taste intensity scores in the right and left anterior parts of the tongue at month 6 compared to preoperative in LFIO patients (p < 0.05). CONCLUSION: Taste perception is affected due to potential damage to the chemosensory nerves during orthognathic surgical procedures. Generally, non-significant alterations have been observed in taste perception after orthognathic surgery, except for significant alterations in bitter and sweet taste perceptions. CLINICAL RELEVANCE: Maxillofacial surgeons should be aware of taste perception change after orthognathic surgery procedures and patients should be informed accordingly. THE TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER (TRN): NCT06103422/Date of registration: 10.17.2023 (retrospectively registered).


Assuntos
Cirurgia Ortognática , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Ortognáticos , Humanos , Mandíbula/cirurgia , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Ortognáticos/métodos , Osteotomia de Le Fort , Osteotomia Sagital do Ramo Mandibular/métodos , Quinina , Paladar , Percepção Gustatória
5.
Food Res Int ; 181: 114125, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38448103

RESUMO

The perception of orosensory stimuli, which includes flavor, can vary between individuals. These individual variations in oral sensations can be due to genetic factors and it would appear that they can predict food liking and consumption. The most studied source of variation is related to bitter taste perception associated with 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) responsiveness. In this context, humans can be classified as non-tasters (NT), medium tasters (MT) and supertasters (ST). Evidence suggests that genetic variation in bitter taste perception contributes to differences in the level of irritation caused by alcohol perception in solutions. The aim of this investigation was to study the bitter taste sensitivity among a group of mezcal consumers and its relationship with sensory perception and preference through PROP taster status. The tests were carried out in the state of Oaxaca in Mexico. A total of 83 mezcal consumers were classified by their PROP taster status and were asked to provide sensory descriptors for five mezcal samples and rate them according to the level of liking. The three-solution test was used to classify the subjects as NT, MT, and ST, while a Multiple Factor Analysis (MFA) was used to visualize the sensory descriptors provided by these three groups. The proportion of MT subjects was 16%, while the proportion of NT and ST was 34 and 51%, respectively. The MT provided higher liking ratings for at least three mezcal samples. According to MFA, the mezcal samples were organized in a similar configuration along the two dimensions. However, NT mentioned a limited number of simple terms (strong flavor, tasteless, burning in the mouth) to describe the samples, whereas ST used a more complex vocabulary (astringent, smoky, scratchy aftertaste). These data suggest that the preference for mezcal samples was similar for non-taster and supertasters, but there are indications that the sensory perception of mezcal differs between groups.


Assuntos
Percepção Gustatória , Paladar , Humanos , Sensação , Adstringentes , Emoções
6.
Int J Mol Sci ; 25(5)2024 Mar 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38474290

RESUMO

Commercial papaya varieties grown in Australia vary greatly in taste and aroma. Previous profiling has identified undesirable 'off tastes' in existing varieties, discouraging a portion of the population from consuming papayas. Our focus on enhancing preferred flavours led to an exploration of the genetic mechanisms and biosynthesis pathways that underlie these desired taste profiles. To identify genes associated with consumer-preferred flavours, we conducted whole RNA sequencing and de novo genome assembly on papaya varieties RB1 (known for its sweet flavour and floral aroma) and 1B (less favoured due to its bitter taste and musty aroma) at both ripe and unripe stages. In total, 180,368 transcripts were generated, and 118 transcripts related to flavours were differentially expressed between the two varieties at the ripe stage. Five genes (cpBGH3B, cpPFP, cpSUS, cpGES and cpLIS) were validated through qPCR and significantly differentially expressed. These genes are suggested to play key roles in sucrose metabolism and aromatic compound production pathways, holding promise for future selective breeding strategies. Further exploration will involve assessing their potential across broader germplasm and various growth environments.


Assuntos
Carica , Paladar , Carica/genética , Austrália , Percepção Gustatória , Aromatizantes
7.
Appetite ; 197: 107295, 2024 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38485060

RESUMO

Recent studies have shown that people can believe that unhealthy foods taste better, even if healthy and unhealthy foods are equally as tasty. Specifically, when tasty and unhealthy foods are frequent in one context but rare in another, people perceive unhealthy foods to taste better, even if health and taste are unrelated. Given that people often consume food in one context, the current study investigated whether false beliefs about the health-taste relationship in foods can also occur in just one single context, in which either healthy or unhealthy foods are predominant, when there is no contrasting context where the respective other food is predominant. In two experiments (N = 342), we presented participants with pictures of meals from a single context and varied the frequency of healthy and unhealthy foods between participants. Although healthy and unhealthy foods tasted equally as good, participants believed that (un)healthy foods tasted better when there were more of them. This research demonstrates that health-taste beliefs might be changed by increasing the relative frequency of healthy foods in the environment overall, not by just offering some healthy and tasty foods.


Assuntos
Preferências Alimentares , Paladar , Humanos , Percepção Gustatória , Refeições , Comportamento de Escolha
8.
J Agric Food Chem ; 72(13): 7336-7343, 2024 Apr 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38508871

RESUMO

Molecular docking has been widely applied in the discovery of new sweeteners, yet the interpretation of computational results sometimes remains difficult. Here, the interaction between the T1R2-T1R3 sweet taste receptor and 66 tasting compounds, including 26 sweet, 19 bitter, and 21 sour substances was investigated by batch molecular docking processes. Statistical analysis of the docking results generated two novel methods of interpreting taste properties. Quantitative correlation between relative sweetness (RS) and docking results created a multiparameter model to predict sweetness intensity, whose correlation coefficient r = 0.74 is much higher than r = 0.17 for the linear correlation model between sweetness and binding energy. The improved correlation indicated that docking results besides binding energy contain undiscovered information about the ligand-protein interaction. Qualitative discriminant analysis of different tasting molecules generated an uncorrelated linear discriminant analysis (UDLA) model, which achieved an overall 93.1% accuracy in discriminating the taste of molecules, with specific accuracy for verifying sweet, bitter, and sour compounds reaching 88.0%, 92.1%, and 100%. These unprecedented models provide a unique perspective for interpreting computational results and may inspire future research on sweetener discovery.


Assuntos
Edulcorantes , Paladar , Edulcorantes/química , Simulação de Acoplamento Molecular , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/metabolismo , Percepção Gustatória
9.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 2368, 2024 Mar 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38531860

RESUMO

The perception and appreciation of food flavor depends on many interacting chemical compounds and external factors, and therefore proves challenging to understand and predict. Here, we combine extensive chemical and sensory analyses of 250 different beers to train machine learning models that allow predicting flavor and consumer appreciation. For each beer, we measure over 200 chemical properties, perform quantitative descriptive sensory analysis with a trained tasting panel and map data from over 180,000 consumer reviews to train 10 different machine learning models. The best-performing algorithm, Gradient Boosting, yields models that significantly outperform predictions based on conventional statistics and accurately predict complex food features and consumer appreciation from chemical profiles. Model dissection allows identifying specific and unexpected compounds as drivers of beer flavor and appreciation. Adding these compounds results in variants of commercial alcoholic and non-alcoholic beers with improved consumer appreciation. Together, our study reveals how big data and machine learning uncover complex links between food chemistry, flavor and consumer perception, and lays the foundation to develop novel, tailored foods with superior flavors.


Assuntos
Cerveja , Percepção Gustatória , Cerveja/análise , Aprendizado de Máquina , Comportamento do Consumidor , Paladar
10.
Food Res Int ; 182: 114159, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38519163

RESUMO

For most untrained novices, talking about wine or imagining the smells and flavours of wine is difficult. Wine experts, on the other hand, have been found to have better imagery for wine, and are also more proficient in describing wine. Some scholars have suggested that imagery and language are based on similar underlying processes, but no conclusive evidence has been found regarding mental imagery and language production. In this study, we examined the relationship between imagery and language use in both novices and experts. In an online experiment, wine experts and novices were asked to imagine the colour, smell, taste and mouthfeel of wines in different situations, and were asked to rate the vividness of the imagined experience as well as describe it with words. The results show that experts differ from novices on a number of linguistic measures when describing wine, including the number of words used, the type of words used, the concreteness of those words, and the adjective to noun ratio. Similarly, imagery for wine was more vivid in wine experts compared to novices in the modalities of smell, taste, and mouthfeel, in alignment with previous work. Surprisingly, we found that no single linguistic variable significantly predicted the reported vividness of wine imagery, neither in experts nor in novices. However, the linguistic model predicted imagery vividness better using data from experts compared to novices. Taken together, these findings underscore that imagery and language are different facets of wine cognition.


Assuntos
Vinho , Vinho/análise , Imaginação , Cognição , Percepção Gustatória , Olfato
11.
J Agric Food Chem ; 72(12): 6533-6543, 2024 Mar 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38488059

RESUMO

The research on the umami receptor-ligand interaction is crucial for understanding umami perception. This study integrated molecular simulations, sensory evaluation, and biosensor technology to analyze the interaction between umami peptides and the umami receptor T1R1/T1R3-VFT. Molecular dynamics simulations were used to investigate the dissociation process of seven umami peptides with the umami receptor T1R1/T1R3-VFT, and by calculating the potential mean force curve using the Jarzynski equation, it was found that the binding free energy of umami peptide is between -58.80 and -12.17 kcal/mol, which had a strong correlation with the umami intensity obtained by time intensity sensory evaluation. Through correlation analysis, the dissociation rate constants (0.0126-0.394 1/s) of umami peptides were found to have a great impact on umami perception. The faster the dissociation rate of umami peptides from receptors, the stronger the perceived intensity of the umami taste. This research aims to elucidate the relationship between the umami peptide-receptor interaction and umami perception, providing theoretical support for the exploration of umami perception mechanisms.


Assuntos
Simulação de Dinâmica Molecular , Paladar , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/metabolismo , Percepção Gustatória , Peptídeos/química , Simulação de Acoplamento Molecular
12.
Cogn Sci ; 48(3): e13421, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38500336

RESUMO

The interest in crossmodal correspondences, including those involving sounds and involving tastes, has experienced rapid growth in recent years. However, the mechanisms underlying these correspondences are not well understood. In the present study (N = 302), we used an associative learning paradigm, based on previous literature using simple sounds with no consensual taste associations (i.e., square and triangle wave sounds at 200 Hz) and taste words (i.e., sweet and bitter), to test the influence of two potential mechanisms in establishing sound-taste correspondences and investigate whether either learning mechanism could give rise to new and long-lasting associations. Specifically, we examined an emotional mediation account (i.e., using sad and happy emoji facial expressions) and a transitive path (i.e., sound-taste correspondence being mediated by color, using red and black colored squares). The results revealed that the associative learning paradigm mapping the triangle wave tone with a happy emoji facial expression induced a novel crossmodal correspondence between this sound and the word sweet. Importantly, we found that this novel association was still present two months after the experimental learning paradigm. None of the other mappings, emotional or transitive, gave rise to any significant associations between sound and taste. These findings provide evidence that new crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes can be created by leveraging the affective connection between both dimensions, helping elucidate the mechanisms underlying these associations. Moreover, these findings reveal that these associations can last for several weeks after the experimental session through which they were induced.


Assuntos
Percepção Gustatória , Paladar , Humanos , Som , Emoções
13.
J Food Sci ; 89(3): 1684-1700, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38317409

RESUMO

The retronasal aroma of Baijiu is closely related to its quality and consumer preference. Retronasal detection thresholds (RDTs) of 44 aroma compounds were determined in 46% v/v ethanol using a three-alternative forced-choice procedure, which varied widely and ranged from less than 0.02 to over 1,000,000 µg/L. Nineteen aroma compounds, including ß-phenylethanol, 2,3,5,6-tetramethylpyrazine, dimethyl trisulfide, and 2-methyl-3-(methyldisulfanyl)furan, had RDTs lower than their orthonasal detection thresholds. Power function curves were used to establish correlations between RDTs and partition coefficients for five esters and four pyrazines (R2  = 0.9575, 0.9969, respectively). Twenty-nine aroma compounds had retronasal odor activity values >1 in a soy sauce aroma type Baijiu. Additionally, time-intensity (TI) results suggested that hexanoic acid, ethyl hexanoate, isoamyl acetate, 3-methyl-butanal, and nonanal contributed to a "burst-aroma" of Baijiu. Conversely, TI results attributed the "after-odor" of Baijiu to dimethyl trisulfide, methional, 2,3,5,6-tetramethylpyrazine, ß-phenylethanol, and other compounds. PRACTICAL APPLICATION: This manuscript provides comprehensive information on the retronasal sensory characteristics of aroma compounds in Baijiu. The results obtained may help understand the contribution of aroma compounds to retronasal aroma perception during Baijiu tasting and give helpful information to the Baijiu industry regarding quality control.


Assuntos
Álcool Feniletílico , Sulfetos , Compostos Orgânicos Voláteis , Odorantes/análise , Cromatografia Gasosa-Espectrometria de Massas/métodos , Percepção Gustatória , Compostos Orgânicos Voláteis/análise
14.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(2): e26564, 2024 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38339911

RESUMO

Wine tasting is a very complex process that integrates a combination of sensation, language, and memory. Taste and smell provide perceptual information that, together with the semantic narrative that converts flavor into words, seem to be processed differently between sommeliers and naïve wine consumers. We investigate whether sommeliers' wine experience shapes only chemosensory processing, as has been previously demonstrated, or if it also modulates the way in which the taste and olfactory circuits interact with the semantic network. Combining diffusion-weighted images and fMRI (activation and connectivity) we investigated whether brain response to tasting wine differs between sommeliers and nonexperts (1) in the sensory neural circuits representing flavor and/or (2) in the neural circuits for language and memory. We demonstrate that training in wine tasting shapes the microstructure of the left and right superior longitudinal fasciculus. Using mediation analysis, we showed that the experience modulates the relationship between fractional anisotropy and behavior: the higher the fractional anisotropy the higher the capacity to recognize wine complexity. In addition, we found functional differences between sommeliers and naïve consumers affecting the flavor sensory circuit, but also regions involved in semantic operations. The former reflects a capacity for differential sensory processing, while the latter reflects sommeliers' ability to attend to relevant sensory inputs and translate them into complex verbal descriptions. The enhanced synchronization between these apparently independent circuits suggests that sommeliers integrated these descriptions with previous semantic knowledge to optimize their capacity to distinguish between subtle differences in the qualitative character of the wine.


Assuntos
Web Semântica , Semântica , Humanos , Olfato/fisiologia , Percepção Gustatória , Sensação , Paladar/fisiologia
15.
Physiol Behav ; 276: 114480, 2024 Mar 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38307360

RESUMO

Flavor is perceived through multiple senses, including gustation and olfaction. Previous studies have shown that different sensory qualities that make up flavor are integrated to inform perceptual judgements. Psychophysical work in humans further suggests a prominent role for congruency (i.e., the learnt correspondence between taste and odor components of flavor through eating experience) in shaping multisensory interactions underlying perceptual judgments of flavor. However, eating experience cannot be controlled in humans, and depending on the type of judgement, these studies yielded mixed findings. Here, we used rats to test how experimentally-controlled experience with specific flavor mixtures (OdorA+TasteA and OdorB +TasteB) from weaning to adulthood affects subsequent flavor preference judgements in a series of two-bottle preference tests. In unisensory conditions, animals made odor or taste preference decisions (i.e., OdorA versus OdorB and TasteA versus TasteB, respectively). In multisensory conditions, animals made identical decisions, but the addition of the other modality rendered one solution congruent; the other one incongruent (e.g., OdorA+TasteA versus OdorB+TasteA). The results show that animals effectively learned congruency associations between the taste and smell components of experienced flavor mixtures. Comparing unisensory and multisensory conditions revealed no systematic effect of congruency on the magnitude of flavor preference, but preferences were less variable in multisensory compared to unisensory conditions. Results from a second group of naïve animals further demonstrate that increased reliability of preference judgements in multisensory conditions was independent of experience.


Assuntos
Olfato , Paladar , Humanos , Ratos , Animais , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Percepção Gustatória , Sensação
16.
Food Chem ; 444: 138503, 2024 Jun 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38335677

RESUMO

Reheating chicken soup is a common culinary practice in daily life. To investigate the impact of reheating frequency on the sensory quality of chicken soup, temporal dominance of sensations (TDS) and multi-TDS were used to characterize changes in dominant sensory attributes during consumption. Additionally, E-nose and E-tongue were utilized to analyze differences in aroma and taste profiles. The alterations in sensory properties were further elucidated by analyzing variations in amino acids, volatile compounds. The findings revealed that there was no significant disparity between fresh soup and heating. However, saltiness and umami, as the most prominent dominant characteristics, intensified with increasing reheating cycles. This can be attributed to an elevation in certain amino acids that contribute to umami perception. Conversely, a reduction in some aldehydes weakened the flavor associated with fat and meat components. Moreover, enlarged oil droplets with uneven distribution within heated soup may account for the heightened greasiness sensation.


Assuntos
Galinhas , Paladar , Animais , Percepção Gustatória , Carne/análise , Aminoácidos/análise
17.
PLoS One ; 19(2): e0296697, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38315702

RESUMO

This study investigated the effects of animated food consumption on human psychology. We developed a movable, edible robot and evaluated the participants' impressions induced by the visualization of its movements and eating of the robot. Although several types of edible robots have been developed, to the best of our knowledge, the psychological effects associated with the eating of a robot have not been investigated. We developed a pneumatically driven edible robot using gelatin and sugar. We examined its perceived appearance and the participants' impressions when it was eaten. In the robot-eating experiment, we evaluated two conditions: one in which the robot was moved and one in which it was stationary. Our results showed that participants perceived the moving robot differently from the stationary robot, leading to varied perceptions, when consuming it. Additionally, we observed a difference in perceived texture when the robot was bitten and chewed under the two conditions. These findings provide valuable insights into the practical applications of edible robots in various contexts, such as the medical field and culinary entertainment.


Assuntos
Robótica , Percepção Gustatória , Humanos , Robótica/métodos , Alimentos , Mastigação , Movimento
18.
Food Res Int ; 178: 113899, 2024 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38309898

RESUMO

Whether "gurus", "emperor" or "influencers", wine consumers need references to help them choosing and buying wines. Given the diversity of possible sources (reviews from wine guides, wine merchants'recommendations or back wine label), where should they turn? Are all wine experts consensual in the way they taste wines? Do they differ depending on their domain of expertise: wine makers, sellers, or critics? This study aimed at understanding how experts apprehend wine tasting in relation to their professional objectives. A mixed design strategy combining content analysis of personal narratives of renowned wine experts with semi-structured interviews of wine professionals was used. Results confirmed that wine experts shared a common way of tasting wines, although their objectives and the way they report their wine tasting experiences differ according to their professional activities. Makers transcribe their perceptions by a list of sensory and technical-based terms focused on the absence or presence of wine defects to assess technical acceptability, Sellers are storytellers generating appeal and pleasure for consumers, and Critics write positive and metaphoric reviews to better highlight the best samples they taste. This raises new questions for the industry about the way in which wines are tasted, a routine practice that is internationally followed by wine professionals, but which may need to be renewed. Combining different data sources appeared to be a promising way to gain in-depth information in exploratory research.


Assuntos
Vinho , Vinho/análise , Paladar , Percepção Gustatória , Comportamento do Consumidor , Comércio
19.
Int J Pharm ; 653: 123868, 2024 Mar 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38309485

RESUMO

Extemporaneous preparation (EP) formulation is an attractive strategy to accelerate the formulation development of new chemical entities for first entry into human study. In this work, an EP suspension formulation for a development drug candidate GDC-6599 was successfully developed. The formulation spanned a wide concentration range from 0.1 to 2.0 mg/mL. A non-solubilizing vehicle, 0.6 % (w/v) methylcellulose solution was used to suspend GDC-6599. An aversive agent denatonium benzoate at an extremely low level (6 ppm) was applied as a taste masking agent. This enabled a simple matrix for the analysis of related substances from GDC-6599 during all stability studies. Microcrystalline cellulose at 10 mg/mL concentration was added to the EP formulation to generate a suspension appearance, leading to the success of using a single placebo for matching active formulation at all concentrations. The developed formulation demonstrated excellent homogeneity, sufficient stability and passed microbiological enumeration test. Rinsing performance test demonstrated that greater than 99.8 % amount of drug was successfully recovered by rinsing with water twice, providing guidance for clinical dosing. Biopharmaceutical assessment was conducted by both in silico simulation and in vitro tests. Greater than 90 % bioaccessibility of the EP suspension formulation was obtained via an in vitro system mimicking the human gastrointestinal absorption, consistent with the result from the in silico modeling. The developed EP formulation was successfully used to support the early single ascending dose (SAD) cohorts of GDC-6599 Phase I clinical study. The formulation matrix and assessment workflow developed in this work are generalizable as a platform for EP formulation development of new chemical entities for early phase clinical studies.


Assuntos
Celulose , Absorção Gastrointestinal , Humanos , Composição de Medicamentos , Administração Oral , Percepção Gustatória , Estabilidade de Medicamentos
20.
Food Res Int ; 179: 114009, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38342534

RESUMO

People are generally poor at remembering complex food stimuli, such as wine. While writing a description has been shown to improve memory performance, talking about wine is generally a difficult task for novices. However, giving novices a framework in which to evaluate the wine may help with the memory process. Using a short-term recognition task, this experiment compared different forms of wine evaluation on the to-be-remembered wine sample, using either 1) a classic smell and taste evaluation, 2) a multisensory metaphor selection task with visual, auditory, and tactile metaphors, or 3) a control condition with no writing. Results from 153 participants revealed that recognition performance between the three groups was not significantly different. Secondary analysis revealed that recognition accuracy was correlated with wine liking for the control group, suggesting that in the absence of explicitly evaluating the wine, participants relied on wine liking as a cue for memory. Implications for theory development and applications in wine education are discussed.


Assuntos
Vinho , Humanos , Vinho/análise , Paladar , Metáfora , Percepção Gustatória , Olfato
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...